Home Contents

5. Follow-up Study on Partial Sample of 12 Schools

5.1Criteria for the sample
5.2Investigative instruments for the 12 schools
5.2.1Assessment survey
5.2.1.1"main task"
5.2.1.2theoretical structure of the school landscape
5.2.1.3"carrier" elements and assessment criteria
5.2.2Assessment interview
5.3Design
5.4Results
5.4.1Assessment survey variables
5.4.2Assessment interview variables
5.4.3Comparison of student mean scores for extreme groups
5.4.4Dimensions of ecological culture and students effects
5.4.4.1Student activity
5.4.4.2Student conservation knowledge
5.4.4.3Student environmental motivation
5.4.5Four empirically derived ecological cultures and test of the original hypothesis

The discussion on extrinsic influences on human behavior has never excluded the natural surroundings or other physical forms as factors, however, concrete empirical studies on "who is influenced how by what" are weakly represented.

Moos (1976) describes in one chapter of his book "The Human Context" several physical factors of human behavior - both natural and human made. These factors fall into the categories: weather, architecture, population density and pollution. "Nature" is named as a physical factor but its manifestation is not further defined or addressed. The factors which are explicitly addressed are limited to climate (general), weather (season, air pressure, temperature, wind and humidity) and the phases of the moon. Landscapes, for example, either natural or manipulated through human design and use, similarly, are hardly even mentioned.

Itelson (1974) sums up the relationship between people and landscape as an externalization of culture (the "symbolic" perspective). People and landscape function together, where function is divided into two categories:

  1. Functions that satisfy present needs and preferences (the "instrumental" perspective) and
  2. Functions that (ought to) satisfy needs and preferences (the "ecological" perspective) of the future.

(These functions are characterized as having both aesthetic as well as existential orientations. This distinction is an expression of the significant difference between living and surviving, quality of life and maintaining minimum life requirements. Although aesthetics is acknowledged here, generally it is further neglected as it is terribly difficult to get a handle on.)

Both Moos and Itelson maintain that people are influenced by their surroundings to the extent that certain behaviors are even impossible in surroundings that are not conducive to those behaviors or that pose a conflict. Both recognize the school as an important "surrounding" and do not restrict this to mean the school building and classrooms alone. Both are of the opinion that physical manifestations of the architecture and interior design influence perception, behavior and learning significantly. However, neither of the two go into any detail on physical structures and design on the "outside", on the grounds - within the school landscape - as a potential source of conflict for perception, learning and of ultimate interest, here, behavior.

I have found several further gleanings on the influence of environments, in general, on human behavior from the following subjects:

Research such as these imply strongly that further research on the effects of the design and use of schools (in the sense of a school "culture") is necessary if we really want to understand more about why and how young people (do not) learn responsible environmental behavior.

Our own research on student environmental motivation has given us insight into the social composition of school student populations and is summarized in the preceding chapter.

 

Summary of relevant results from the study on
outcomes of environmental education at school

Analysis of the student variables suggest:

on student environmental motivation

  • 4 motivation profiles for 9th grade students

    Class 1: "HIGH" high scores for all motivation variables; "sensitive optimists"

    Class 2: "MODHI" moderate motivation scores with moderately high self competence; "go with the flow-ers"

    Class 3: "MODLO" moderate motivation scores with moderately low self competence; "self-doubters"

    Class 4: "LO" low scores for all eight motivation variables; "skeptics"

on student environmental activity

  • 4 student profiles for conservation activities taking place at school

    Class 1: "SKL-ACTIV" high participation for at least several of the items

    Class 2: "SKL-LBE" "a little bit of everything"

    Class 3: "SKL-CLEAN" moderate participation in waste related activities

    Class 4: "SKL-LO" low scores for all eight environmental activities

on student knowledge on conservation at

  • Traditional categories according to themes: water conservation, nature conservation, energy conservation, resource conservation, and transportation were replaced with categories that reflect more student contact to the items:
    1. nature conservation at school
    2. environmental conservation at school with high probability for student contact
    3. environmental conservation at school with student contact unlikely
  • Student knowledge is most accurate for category "2" followed by "1" and then "3" suggesting that student contact plays a role in knowledge. Students were least certain of their knowledge ("I don’t know") for category " 3" and most certain for "2".

on motivation and activity

  • There appears to be a correlation between student motivation and educational student activity opportunities in conservation suggesting that environmental motivation can be characterized by the types of school activities students perform.
  • "sensitive optimists" show the least preference for school activities and appear to be least impacted by activity opportunities presented at school;
  • "go with the flow" students are most likely to be "environmentally active" at school appear to be influenced by the environmental activities offered there;
  • "self-doubters" most likely perform clean-up activities and least likely to respond that they do little or nothing appear to be influenced by school activity opportunities;
  • "skeptics" see themselves as least active and least likely to belong to the "active" group. They, nonetheless, appear, to be influenced by school activity opportunities.

on motivation and knowledge

  • Students with high motivation tend to have better knowledge about their school’s nature conservation and environmental conservation; students with low motivation have less knowledge. This trend is weakened for items on environmental conservation where student contact is likely. This suggests that the school (contact) has a moderating influence on student knowledge which is stronger than preferences attributable to student motivation.

Analysis of school variables:

on features/ modifications and student activity opportunities

  • It was not possible to make satisfactory qualitative distinctions among school ecological profiles. Differences were at best quantitative.
  • Data on student activity opportunities were too sparse to establish school profiles.

on school variables and motivation

  • No explanations possible which explain student differences.

Table 5-1: Summary of relevant results from the study on outcomes of environmental education at school (1997)

Results suggest that not only does the social structure (with regard to environmental motivation) vary from school to school, but that there may well be conditions at schools that effect student environmental motivation in general. In particular, school effects on students tend to be seen most noticeably in their responses to items on coping style, self-competence and social norm.

It is conceivable that if conditions can be enhanced that student preferences and needs in acquiring environmental behaviors may be satisfied better by the schools.

Since all 54 schools in our previous survey report conservation effort s to some degree. This confirms an overall investment in and commitment to conservation. However, our findings have neither detected differences among schools nor been able to explain even in part differences in student environmental motivation. For instance, we have found no evidence to support a popular belief that "more conservation is better" or that a broad variety of activities fosters, for example, student knowledge. It still remains a mystery which possibilities are available to schools that can be implemented for the benefit of students.

In addition to a lack of understanding on the school conditions at play, there appears to be substantial discrepancies between student and administration responses on just what is going on in conservation at their schools. Student responses indicate that they are not aware of many conservation efforts undertaken at their school (especially for energy and water conservation). Conversely, for a number of students at many of the schools there are conservation efforts going on that the administration is not aware of. It appears, then, that schools have greater resources at hand for acquainting students with conservation and practicing conservation skills than they currently employ. But better utilization of school resources - more activity - alone is not expected to bridge the educational gap and promote favorable outcomes in environmental behavior. We presently lack understanding on 1) which school activities are effective in meeting which motivational needs of which students; as well as 2) which perspectives, policies, practices and opportunities in conservation at the school lend authenticity to the cultural landscape.

The evident student differences but shortcomings on information on schools, the above mentioned considerations and a moderate sense of pride have lead to further investigation into the matter. The course I attempt to take here addresses the claim that school environmental conservation is an evident part of environmental education but one which is not reaching the students much of the time. Education for the conservation of nature - or for the protection of the environment - is at least in part a question of culture and cannot be left to a special event, an environmental phase or good intentions to get the job done. Special behaviors require special settings if they are to be performed habitually.

The follow-up study attempts to improve upon the deficits of the study on outcomes of environmental education at school with an extension of the educational model for environmental behavior to include the identification and measure of elements of an "ecological culture" of schools.

The integrated model for environmental action (Rost, 1995) makes up the dependent variable block in both the follow-up study and the survey on environmental education outcomes. See Figures 4-5, 5-1 and 5-2. New items on elements of an ecological school culture have been constructed for the independent variables.

Figure 5-1: Survey design on outcomes of environmental education at school (1996)

Figure 5-2: Follow-up study design (1997)

In this follow-up study, as in environmental psychology (Bronfenbrenner, 1979), each culture includes both objects and spaces, on the one hand, and practices or ways of doing things - with objects or with people, on the other. Vygotzki (1987) elaborates upon this by adding a behavioral aspect which defines culture as a continual process of exchange between collective and subjective meanings. Reinterpreted for the "ecological culture" of schools, this would imply that:

An ecological school culture includes both objects and spaces and school related practices or ways of doing things with objects or with people. An ecological school culture is a continual process of exchange between collective and subjective meanings of conservation at school of all persons involved.

Now, it is beyond the scope of this follow-up study to try and capture the process of exchange between states of shared and subjective meaning students go through during the learning process. However, we can try and extract a portion of a collective meaning of conservation at school. This I have represented with the collective learning experiences that are offered to the students as well as by standard practices stated by the administration. This collective meaning, accordingly, cannot be operationalized explicitly as one variable. It ought to be, rather, functionally equivalent with the environmental culture of a school.

The new educational model for environmental behavior here attempts to explain the influence of an ecological culture at school on student motivation to act environmentally in terms of four kinds of independent variables.

It is expected, therefore, that:

  1. a school with a marked "ecological culture" has identifiable structural cues; and
  2. the "ecological culture" is reflected in its students in terms of knowledge (of conservation at their school), student environmental activity there and, ultimately, environmental motivation such that:
    1. environmental practices at school, when a part of the school culture, or habitualized, reduce the aversions and obstacles in performing them;
    2. a pronounced ecological culture at school fulfills student needs and preferences thereby promoting the acknowledgment of the necessity of environmental protection, options for solutions, necessary skills, social infrastructure (norms) and, in turn, motivation to act environmentally;
    3. participation opportunities in conservation are most likely to be motivational when student participation in the decision and planning process is esteemed and not merely seen as a favor or luxury; and
    4. school standing conveys the message that conservation is worthwhile and reinforces practical conservation at the school.

In summary, the central idea to be investigated here is: a school with a pronounced ecological culture has greater student participation in environmental activities, more of its students are knowledgeable of conservation there and they are significantly more environmentally motivated.

The next sections describe the sample, design and instruments of the follow-up.

Home Contents

5.1. Criteria for the sample

The follow-up study was intended to investigate a way of distinguishing schools which might lead to a plausible explanation for the students differences we observed during the study on outcomes of environmental education at school in 1996.

As it was decided that only twelve of the 54 original schools could be revisited, criteria were needed to choose among them to assure that better information would be collected on their "ecological culture". With the empirical study as my basis, the following criteria were generated for the schools participating in the follow-up study:

Table 5-2 compares means scores and range for dependent variables for the follow-up group of twelve schools with the sample of 54.

A comparison of the follow-up and sample groups
on the distribution of student motivation

 

"sensitive optimists"


(MOT1)

"go with the flowers"
active coping style and higher self-competence
(MOT2)

"self doubters"
passive coping style and lower self-competence
(MOT3)

"skeptics"


(MOT4)

1) sample of 54 schools in Germany (students=2365)

Average=

26.7%

26.1%

26.1%

21.1%

Range=

8.3 - 46.3%

7.1 - 62.5%

7.5 - 50.0%

4.2 - 42.6%

2) follow-up study at 12 city schools (n=591)

Average=

25.3%

24.8%

27.0%

22.9%

Range=

8.9 - 41.5%

7.1 - 43.1%

13.9 - 41.7%

8.7 - 41.7%

Table 5-2: A comparison of the follow-up group of 12 and the sample of 54 schools

This table shows mean scores on student motivation of the follow-up group is comparable to those of the sample group. The range is similar for the two extreme motivation profiles but somewhat narrower for the two moderate motivation profiles.

The following section will describe the two instruments (assessment survey and interview) and their development for use in assessing ecological culture at schools. The section that follows will briefly describe the investigative design.

Home Contents

5.2. Investigative instruments for the 12 schools

In addition to measurable health benefits for students and faculty at a school which observes environmentally sustainable practices, I anticipate effects in student learning, motivation and behavior. The results of a study in England on the effects of school grounds (Titman, 1994; Adams, 1990; Department of Education and Science, 1990) suggest evidence significant for student effects on attitudes and behavior. The results of their study indicate that:

"Learning through Landscapes", as Titman’s project is known, suggests that a school and its grounds can be read just as a landscape can. The landscape gives cues on, for example, what behaviors are appropriate and offers students a "training course" of sorts. The students "read" and interpret the school landscape cues and learn.

Whether the school is seen by students or by an observer, the individual elements of the "landscape" result in a unique landscape image for each individual and not in a purely tangible landscape which is identical for all (Hoisl, 1992). Despite this, I require instruments of investigation that can "read" the school landscape in a way that is methodologically sound and practical to use (objective), yet, still relevant to understanding impacts on students. I have found no previously used tool for doing this, so an important stage of this investigation became the development of the necessary assessment instruments.

All data for each school would be collected on site and must be completed within one school day by one person. Students or teachers could not be observed. Two instruments were chosen: an assessment survey and an assessment interview.

The objective behind the assessment survey is to have an instrument which can measure "ecological culture" objectively, quantifiably. It is not the goal of this assessment to be able to say school "X" requires "Y" square meters more green space. Rather this research attempts to identify relevant variables, categorize schools and explain observed differences among students attending these schools. The categories for scoring the assessment survey were chosen to be readily understood, convenient to use and readily transferable for multiple "landscapes". In addition to methodological advantages, the practicality of the assessment is necessary to minimize expenses, effort and time incurred during its use.

(Also, it is intended that the instrument be flexible to accommodate new criteria or categories for a new assessment task. It enables the investigation of more than one objective and means that the results are transferable for more than one reference point (objective), for use with more than one student population and for altered "landscapes" (those altered at a later date).)

But as already mentioned, not only are all cues are not visible cues, they are not necessarily tangible in the sense that they can be seen, felt, heard or smelled (the only means available to the assessment survey). This was the reason for choosing a second instrument, the assessment interview. The interview attempts to get an initial perspective on some non-visible components of the ecological school culture, namely: How are things done there at the school? What are the intended messages that the school administration wants to convey? How do they (intend to) do this? Are there "hidden" messages that may interfere with the intended ones? To ensure that the responses from different schools could be compared, a categorial answer format was provided and strongly encouraged.

Data collected from both new instruments on independent school variables will then be compiled with data collected previously on independent student variables (study on environmental education outcomes) as described in the section on Design.

The next two sections describe the two assessment instruments in more detail.

Home Contents

5.2.1. Assessment survey

If we are allowed to regard the public area beyond the walls of the classroom and within the boundaries of the school property as a defined and designed environment, then this may also be referred to as a man-made habitat or landscape. As a man-made or cultural landscape, the school grounds is the product of part objective part subjective part collective representation. My goal here is to develop an instrument to assess the ecological culture as they are represented at schools.

I have found no documentation on guidelines, previous attempts or instruments for the measurement and evaluation of school buildings and grounds appropriate for this investigation. My concept for the measure and evaluation of school "landscapes" has been developed from work done by Gabriela Leitl et al. (1997) at the "Bureau for Ecological Planning, Environmental Studies and Services" in Bayreuth, Germany.

The interplay and, in specific, the influence of landscapes on people - especially on perception and behavior - is a central theme of the work of city and regional planers. They have the task, among others, to preserve and in some cases restore the uniqueness of landscapes as source for identity and belongingness of the residents and at the same time protect, preserve ("maintain") and develop nature within these landscapes (Abs. 1 Nr. 4 Bundesnaturschutzgesetz, Federal Nature Protection Law).

In 1997 a concept was presented for the measurement and evaluation of landscapes. In general, Leitl’s concept proposes that each landscape has a unique and describable structure. The structure is made up of a set of "carrier" elements more or less bound together in a predictable way characterizing the given landscape. It is these carrier elements that can then be assessed and compiled. On the basis of the assessment, the landscape can then be categorized in terms useful in fulfilling the "main task" or function, in this case, of the city/ regional planner defined by the Federal law described above.

Leitl’s concept is designed to be transferable to various landscapes, but unfortunately not readily so for my purposes. Aside from the fact that the function I am interested in is different, one must bare in mind that the concept from Leitl et al. is applicable to areas generally much larger in scale than school grounds and that consequently, for their purposes, measurement and evaluation takes place at a "lower resolution". A "school" and its property would most likely be recognized and included as an item of measure but not further differentiated. Therefore, an assessment of schools at the level of scale in the follow-up study requires an elaboration on the theoretical structure. (A structure at a level not yet defined in theory and my first challenge.)

In the next few paragraphs I will attempt to make clear how I adapted Leitl’s model for the assessment of schools. The ideas that will be described are:

  1. "main task" of a school assessment on ecological culture
  2. theoretical structure describing a school "landscape"
  3. "carrier" elements and criteria of an assessment survey
  4. resulting assessment categories of ecological culture at schools

Home Contents

5.2.1.1. "Main task"

The first step is to define the "main task" or function of the school "landscape". In search of this definition I found a reference to the "Second Dialog on Environment and Schools in Lower Saxony ("2. Schulische Umweltgespräche in Niedersachsen", Hesse, 1996). Here it was identified that:

the main task in the "maintenance, care and design of school buildings and grounds" is to create a place of well-being and promote a sense of responsibility in students.

This goal, interestingly enough, was previously formulated as the promotion of human and environmental compatibility (Ministry of Education of Lower Saxony, see above). The reformulation records a shift from the goal to preserve environmental and human integrity (details not specified) to the goal to design for a particular setting or habitat. This semantic refinement suggests a definite degree of development of the idea that external physical surroundings are malleable and therefore can be designed intentionally to effect a particular and desirable influence on student behavior.

So, even though one school landscape is much like another in that they both contain similar objects such as sports fields, lawns, paths, vegetation, play equipment or biotic, abiotic, and man-made "micro", "meso" and "macro" structures, they can hardly be considered identical. Because they are not identical, there must obviously exist alternative expressions for these objects resulting in the unique "landscapes" we experience. To "read" how well the school landscape fulfills its task, the individual objects are organized within a structure described in the following section.

Home Contents

5.2.1.2. Theoretical structure of the school landscape

Similar to the idea of "well-being", the purpose of the school grounds is repeatedly described as the fulfillment of student needs or the fulfillment of functions (Brunner, 1997; Moore, 1980, Bastian, 1991). This subject has already been addressed in the Introduction and Historical Background of this report. The school grounds, alternatively, can be viewed as a collection of individual activities which fall into one of several categories. The activity categories that were chosen and serve simultaneously as "carrier" elements of the function of the school landscape are:

Each school is assessed for a structure based on the prevalence and character of these five "carrier" elements.

Home Contents

5.2.1.3. "Carrier" elements and assessment criteria

In contrast to regional planning, it cannot be individual topographical or geographical elements alone that make a (school) landscape what it is. In the case of schools we are primarily interested here in cumulative effect of all elements for the entire student body. The cumulative effect may manifest itself in different ways and result in influencing motivation differently for different kinds of kids.

I have described the school landscape in terms of four "carrier" elements which characterize distinct student activity needs. Table 5-3 typifies the indicators used to identify these types of activities, or "carrier" elements.


Table 5-3: Activity indicators for "carrier" elements for school landscape functions

It is possible for this assessment survey but hardly helpful to count individual trees or keep track of the square meters of lawn etc. There is certainly, for example, a minimum space requirement for the functionality of areas with respect to number of students or age or... This subject and similar ones go beyond the scope of this research at this time. Instead, the indicators ought to be a measure of the schools ability to meet student needs.

The degree to which the school landscape accommodates each element is a measure of the quality of student experiences in satisfying their activity needs and is ultimately and ideally a measure of the well-being of students there.

"Accommodation" is not just a matter of a checklist indicating that the indicator is there. "I see it" or "I do not". Rather, human perception - of students and assessor alike - includes more than the sense of sight alone and thus the influence of a landscape on people is not merely visual. Not to mention the fact that visual representations of the same element will be perceived differently by different people. Particularly for young people, other sources of stimulation are important. These include the acoustic, olfactory and tactile aspects of experience. For example, it is not likely that the sound of birds has the same effect as traffic sounds or the smell of wild flowers compared with exhaust fumes or the feel of earth compared with concrete. Ideally, the assessment ought to reflect this.

Factors to be considered for the assessment of the four "carrier" elements have been adapted and are depicted in Table 5-4.


Table 5-4: Factors considered in assessing "carrier" elements for school landscapes

In this investigation both individual as well as cumulative, or contextual, effects are regarded as instrumental in creating a landscape and influencing student motivation. This table shows that the elements of school landscapes are assessed for two types of attributes. Included are attributes which assess the element on its own merits or inherent attributes as well as those which assess the element in context or in relationship to things external to the element.

Differences in interpretation are unavoidable, however, they ought to be quantitative and not qualitative in nature and, therefore, to avoid unnecessary differences in interpretation clearly quantifiable categories for assessment are necessary. The assessment criteria are those qualities identified as relevant to the "main task" (see section 5.2.1.1) put in quantifiable terms. Particularly this point has been a special challenge in this investigation.

I am striving at a measure of "things" that for the sake of the kind of statistical analysis I would like to conduct must be recorded in objective and quantifiable terms. Historically, though, these "things" have been recorded - if at all - more often than not subjectively or qualitatively. This data has limited value for empirical statistical analysis - a problem which I am now trying to overcome.

For each attribute there is a defined set of criteria which enables the classification of the indicator into one of three simple categories which characterize its prevalence as "high", "average" or "low". To give an idea of this a very small portion on the list of assessment criteria is shown in Table 5-5.

Table 5-5: Sample criteria for assessing individual attributes of one "carrier" element

Each "carrier" element has its own set of criteria for the assessment of the attributes. This table presents only a very small portion of a very extensive list of criteria used to assess the "carrier" elements. During four practice runs it was determined that this strategy was far too intensive, in fact redundant, and hard to manage in relationship to the results it produced. It becomes very complicated, for example, to assess attributes if the elements topographically overlap as is frequent for "non-structured activity" and "nature experience" areas.

To maintain clarity and preserve the informative value of the assessment, it was decided instead to re-focus on six general dimensions of an ecological school landscape. Indeed these dimensions do cover many of the elements and attributes previously described but using dimensions in place of "carrier" elements, I have attempted to reduce the amount of assessment attributes to a minimum and at the same time improve the usefulness of the information. The dimensions of school landscapes which were finally used are:

I would like to make some overall remarks on the individual on these dimensions or traits as they will later be called.

The attributes used to evaluate these dimensions/ traits remain for the most part identical to those worked out previously for the "carrier" elements, only they have been condensed in number and regrouped for the appropriate dimension. A summary of the dimensions and their corresponding attributes is given in Table 5-6. A translation of the original assessment survey is included in the Appendix.

Table 5-6: Dimensions and attributes for assessing school landscapes

This table depicts the final dimensions and their attributes used in assessing school landscapes. At this stage the assessment is complete. An interpretation of the assessment survey results are discussed in section 5.4.1.

Home Contents

5.2.2. Assessment interview

The assessment survey is concerned with variables on ecological features and environmental modifications of the school grounds, where as the assessment survey is concerned with the remaining variables on:

It has been mentioned already mentioned, that in order for it to work, education and learning for responsible environmental behavior ought to be going on everywhere as a normal part of doing things - a standard and not a hobby. But such practices and student involvement take on a variety of forms that are not readily identifiable - not even to the trained eye. Furthermore, the assessment survey is not designed for observing students and faculty directly. Thus, I have not found it possible to "see" general practices or student participation in conservation at school in the sense that I can identify it in a form suitable for the assessment survey (5.2.1). Therefore, a second instrument, the assessment interview, was used in the identification of ecological culture at schools. The interview is intended to collect data on the ecological culture which cannot be observed or might be potentially "overlooked". Examples of questions I cannot answer with the assessment survey are: How are things done there at the school? What are the intended messages that the school administration wants to convey? How do they do this? (through student participation, management and organization, as well as through design) Are there "hidden" messages that may interfere with the intended ones?

In section 5.2.1.1 two conditions were named that would be necessary if student well-being and their taking on responsibilities were to be genuine goals of the school. These goals can only be attained when:

  1. the school takes on a positive role and lifestyle which includes the design of the school and person-to-person relationships as well as learning content, methods and technical support; and
  2. the school upholds dialogue and cooperation with politicians, education agencies and other institutions on the needs of youth.

School practices, student participation, "lifestyle", student-faculty relationships and dialogue with others, then, formed the core themes of the interview which contained more than 20 in depth items. Questions that summarize the themes addressed by the assessment interview are:

  1. Who has initiated conservation efforts concerning ecological features and environmental modifications at the school?
  2. Through which means do students experience the meaning of nature and environmental conservation?
  3. (How) Are the grounds and the buildings integrated in nature and environmental conservation?
  4. What possibilities for conservation are offered to students?
  5. What is student behavior in conservation like, in general? Which "effects" of environmental education have been observed in students? Where does work need to be done?
  6. How is all this organized (conservation/ student participation) at the school?
  7. How is conservation at the school presented to the outside?

A small sample of the assessment interview questions and the answer format is given below. This is an approximate translation. The original questions and answer format are more elaborate. This answer format represents the resulting categories.

Table 5-7: A small sample of the assessment interview questions and answer format

The interview was held with the principal of the school or someone appointed by them and was geared to last not longer than one class period (45 minutes). To assure that the responses from different schools could be compared and facilitate completion, the interview was held in a structured format. Questions were pre-formulated, similarly presented and a categorial answer format was offered and strongly encouraged to the interviewees. All responses were recorded on paper immediately and simultaneously tape-recorded. The assessment interview is found in the Appendix.

Home Contents

5.3. Design

Approximately one year after a survey on student outcomes on environmental education at school was conducted, a follow-up investigation took place. The follow-up was not intended to re-test the schools, rather, it was intended to gather more data on the independent variables on traits of an ecological school "culture". Twelve of the original 54 schools were revisited. The schools were chosen from the original sample such that the data from both studies could be compiled together. Significant changes at the school between testing times were accounted for.

Schools were considered for the sample after analysis of three factors 1) demographics, 2) conservation and student activities and 3) motivation subpopulations. A more detailed description is given in section 5.1. I requested the voluntary participation of twelve specifically chosen schools. No school declined cooperation. A day was arranged during early Spring for the assessment survey and interview. The entire visit ran on average five hours.

Students or teachers would not be observed or interviewed in the follow-up study. Instead, four new categories of variables on school traits were under investigation:

The data on these new variables were collected using two methods (Section 5.2). Data on the ecological manifestations were collected during an assessment "inspection" or survey of the grounds and buildings using an assessment criterion catalogue and during an assessment interview with the school principal.

The criterion catalogue consists of over 40 items (found in the Appendix), is intended to be carried out both indoors and outdoors and includes aspects, such as, layout, design, surfaces, condition, nature and environmental conservation and student access to natural settings.

Data on practices and in particular on student involvement or "participation" as it seems to be referred to more often today, was obtained through more than 20 items during interviews with the school principals or someone appointed by them. The principals were not given the questions prior to the interview. They were, however, given an oral report on our major results from the original study along with a detailed summary of the results of their school.

The design of the assessment is shown below.

Figure 5-3: Design for the assessment of ecological school culture

In its simplest form, results from individual schools are expected to take the form of one of the quadrants in Table 5-8.

Table 5-8: Anticipated ecological cultures at schools measured by conservation features and practices

This table depicts a generalized range of results expected from the twelve schools. It is anticipated that schools will tend to score high or low for each of the assessment components (i.e., features or student participation). The resulting profile (one of four) is interpreted as the ecological culture of that school.

The "old" data on the dependent variables on student motivation can then be reinterpreted using, now, the additional, newly collected data on the independent variables on ecological school culture. It is anticipated that the new instruments are more sensitive and the data better such that the ecological school culture can now be reflected in a characteristic make-up of student subpopulations on environmental motivation typical for that ecological culture profile.

For example, it is expected that there are schools that score proportionately high for most or all measures of ecological culture. These "strong" schools, it is postulated, ought to differ from other schools in a number of ways. For instance, proportionately more students at these schools ought to belong to the highest scoring environmental motivation group, have higher than average knowledge of conservation going on at their school and report more experience in environmental activities at (and away from) school than the remaining students.

Or, another category of school is anticipated which scores comparatively low for ecological culture. It is postulated that a significantly larger portion of the student body at these schools will belong more likely than not to either of the lowest scoring motivation groups, score significantly lower for their accuracy on conservation at school and report lower activity at and school than the others.

Similarly there are two potential mixed forms of school ecological cultures that may be encountered. Schools that either score high for features and low for participation. These I have labeled "standard" as I feel this is the constellation characteristic of most schools. Or there may be schools that do not put emphasis on conservation features at all, but nonetheless give the student participation process high priority. These I have named "other" to reflect that these schools perhaps share a similar value for education through participation, but have chosen an alternative "landscape". Instead of "conservation" underpinning participatory activity at school, it could be, for example, "social democracy" or "health".

The next section describes the successes and failures I encountered investigating ecological culture at school and its effect on student environmental motivation using the two assessment instruments and data previously collected.

Home Contents

5.4. Results

Data on schools were collected in 1996 as part of an empirical study on outcomes of environmental education at school and one year later, in 1998, as part of a follow-up study on ecological school culture. The follow-up study comprised twelve of the original 54 German public schools and was intended to produce better data on the independent variables on ecological cultures at schools. Although data on students were not collected during the follow-up survey, the data on and from students collected from the survey in 1996 could be combined with the data collected in 1997. The combined databases include the twelve schools and about 590 9th graders. The improved database is intended to help explain school effects of ecological culture on students, in particular, on student environmental motivation.

The follow-up investigation was carried out using two instruments: an assessment survey and an assessment interview - both of which were specially developed for this task (section 5.2). The next sections give a general description of the schools in the sample, describe structures found among schools and then propose traits of ecological culture at schools that may explain differences in student motivation.

An outline follows of the results we shall be seeing:

Descriptives:

  1. Description of survey variables on features and modifications in conservation and resulting categories; and
  2. Description of the interview variables on conservation practices and resulting categories. And on
  3. Structures:

  4. Comparison of mean scores for the dependent variables (student activity at school, conservation knowledge and environmental motivation) for two extreme school groups; and
  5. Description of four empirically derived ecological cultures and test of the original hypothesis.

A review of the investigative design reminds us that the twelve German public schools in the follow-up study are all found in cities with populations of at least 75,000 in four "old" federal states ("West Germany"). They represent a broad spectrum of conservation practices and student activity opportunities found at public schools here.

School participation was voluntary. No school declined cooperation. A day was arranged at each school for both the assessment survey and interview. The entire visit ran on average five hours. Students or teachers were not observed or interviewed in the follow-up study, rather, data was collected on the ecological culture at schools (independent variables):

Now we will look at the individual variables collected by the assessment instruments.

Home Contents

5.4.1. Assessment survey variables

The assessment survey consists of over 40 items (found in the Appendix), is intended to be carried out both indoors and outdoors and includes items on aspects such as: layout, design, surfaces, condition, nature and environmental conservation and student access to natural settings.

Although data was collected both inside and out (on school grounds and on school buildings), data from the assessment survey inside the schools did not add particularly to my understanding and, as a consequence, have been left out here for the sake of simplicity. Table 5-9 below is an approximate translation of the assignment categories used in evaluating the traits of ecological features at schools.


Table 5-9: Three Assignment Categories for Traits on Ecological Features

In this table the three assignment categories should represent a range of prevalence reasonable for each trait. The category level indicates the anticipated value that trait in that form has on ecological culture.

For further evaluation of the criteria on traits, it is important that for each variable all categories are represented in our sample. Traits which have missing categories must be eliminated from the assessment catalogue - at least for this sample. Table 5-10 gives a summary of the number of responses for each response category.

 

Table 5-10: Assessment Survey Variables on Ecological Features and School Assignment to Three Levels

The next step in the evaluation was, for each variable, to assign the schools to groups according to the level at which that variable is present. For traits measured with only one item, the level was predetermined by the categorial answer format. For traits measured with multiple items, scales were built and the upper third was assigned to Level 3, the middle third was assigned to Level 2 and the lower third was assigned to Level 1.

This table shows that each category is represented at least once suggesting that the category levels are reasonable in representing a range of trait prevalence among the twelve schools in the sample.

Due to the small sample size and the effects of aggregation, as has already been mentioned, I cannot expect my data to verify theory. I will not see strong effects from twelve aggregate scores and I cannot rely on significance tests due to the small sample size. I can still, however, compare these schools among themselves and see which traits, if any, seem to be associated with student effects. I can do this by comparing student data (student "effects") for the extreme groups - Levels 1 and 3. The main objective, then, was to build school groups that are distinguishable relative to each other and not to distinguish schools based on a purely quantitative measure or raw score.

A similar procedure was followed for the evaluation of the variables collected with the assessment interview discussed next.

Home Contents

5.4.2. Assessment interview variables

Data on general environmental practices at school and student involvement or "participation" in conservation (as it seems to be referred to more often today), was obtained through more than 20 interview items. The interview was held with the school’s principal or someone appointed by them. The principals were not given the questions prior to the interview. They were, however, given an oral report on our major results from the original study along with a detailed summary of the results of their school.

Almost all principals appreciated this very much and seemed to gain a better understanding for what I mean when I say "conservation", "student participation" and "motivation". I feel this preparation improved our communication for the interview.

The interview lasted between one and two hours depending on how communicative the principal was. Sometimes interruptions caused delays. But for the most part all principals were very cooperative about sharing their experiences and following the prescribed answer format. All twelve interviews were carried through to completion and without hindrances worth mentioning. A very condensed summary of the interview variables has been translated and reconstructed in Table 5-11.


Three Assignment Levels for traits on General Practices and Student Activity Opportunities in Conservation at School

(a translation)

Item (variable)

Level 1

(-)

Level 2

Level 3

(+)

General conservation practices (gp)

How much time is allotted to them to spend outside? (gp1)

  • no more than half an hour
  • ½ - 1 hour per day
  • more than an hour

General practices: recycling, packaging, waste, paper (gp2)

  • all areas low
  • intermediate or in only one area high
  • all areas high

Are your students involved? (gp3)

  • not particularly
  • some of them
  • most of them

What percentage of the students come to school (gp4)

  • >20% with parents and <50 public
  • in between
  • <10% with parents and >=60% public

As a result of activities at school, students: (gp5)

  • little change if any
  • minimal (sort waste produce less waste)
  • at least moderate change noticeable

Our school could do more to improve (rank) in students (gp6)

  • little or no improvement needed
  • moderate improvements
  • lots to do

For activities and the grounds, do you have resources for (gp7)

  • inadequate
  • yes, but limited
  • if I need it I got it

General student activity opportunities in conservation (sa)

Who initiated conservation or design of grounds at school? (sa1)

  • no one yet, anyone could theoretically
  • only faculty
  • school based group including students

Rank the experiences students at your school attain through conservation activity. (sa2)

  • pract. exp. competence
  • social competence
  • cognitive rational thinking
  • emot. competence
  • communication skills

Is there a long term plan for school grounds development? (sa3)

  • no plan
  • faculty and external (no students)
  • and student partic. on plan, implem, maint, eval

What sort of activity opportunities are offered? (sa4)

  • plan., implem., or less
  • plan., implem., maintenance
  • "everything"

Students in the 9th grade (sa5)

  • were chosen (entire grade)
  • were chosen (particular course)
  • could volunteer freely

Is there an environmental codex for all persons at the school? (sa6)

  • no
  • faculty participation only
  • teachers/ students assigned

Is conservation at school documented? (sa7)

  • no particular effort
  • effort is made
  • effort made with students

General standing on conservation at school (o)

What is school’s role in conveying importance of conservation as compared with other sources? (o1)

  • little, insignificant
  • moderate
  • decisive

Which resources are important for educating on conservation? (o2)

  • "instruction" highest importance
  • "instruction" equal to others
  • "instruction" secondary

Conservation activity provides valuable experiences. Rank for which areas this occurs. (o3)

  • recreational
  • scientific, nature, env.
  • social, personal develop.
  • occupational

The school grounds and facilities ought to (priority): (o4)

  • support classroom instruction low
 
  • all high

The design of the grounds ought to fulfill (rank): (o5)

  • except for sport, one category "no"
  • 1 category (except sport) "w/reservation"
  • all categories recognized

gp=general practice (gp1-7)

sa= student activity (sa1-7)

o=other, stand to school and conservation (01-5)

Table 5-11: Assessment Interview Variables on General Practices and Student Participation in Conservation at School

Similar to Table 5-8 on traits of ecological features collected with the assessment survey, this table gives the items and levels for many of the interviewed traits on general practices, student participation opportunities in conservation at school and additional items on the administrations’ standing to school and conservation ("o") in general. This is not the interview and answer format. This is a summary of the resulting categories after responses were evaluated and the three levels had been assigned. The assignment categories, in this case, do represent the range of prevalence for each trait in our sample. The category level indicates the anticipated value that trait in that form has on ecological culture.

The pre-structured answer format was very helpful for the interpretation of the responses as twelve interviewees can often lead to at least thirteen different structures for interpretation. All principals adhered to the given answer format with the exception of one who did not want to be pinned down on anything.

School Assignment to Three Levels
of traits on General Practices and Student Activity Opportunities in Conservation at School

(a translation)

V

Item

Level 1

(-)

Level 2

Level 3

(+)

gp1

How much time is allotted to them to spend outside?

5

3

4

gp2

General practices: recycling, packaging, waste, paper

3

7

2

gp3

Are your students active/ involved?

7

3

2

gp4

What percentage of the students come to school

2

7

3

gp5

As a result of activities at school, students:

4

4

4

gp6

Our school could do more to improve for students

3

5

4

gp7

For activities and the grounds, do you have resources for

2

4

6

sa1

Who has initiated conservation or design of the grounds at the school?

1

4

7

sa2

Rank the experiences students attain through conservation activity.

2

3

7

sa3

Is there a long term plan for school grounds development?

4

2

6

sa4

What sort of activity opportunities are offered?

2

3

7

sa5

Students in the 9th grade

6

2

4

sa6

Is there an environmental codex for all persons at the school?

5

3

4

sa7

Is conservation at school documented?

5

2

5

o1

School’s role in conveying importance of conservation. (o1)

 

6

6

o2

Which resources are important for educating on conservation? (o2)

3

7

2

o3

Conservation activity provides valuable experiences. Rank them. (o3)

4

4

4

o4

The school grounds and facilities ought to (priority): (o4)

 

3

6

o5

The design of the grounds ought to fulfill (rank): (o5)

4

5

2

gp=general practice (gp1-10)

sa= student activity (sa1-7)

o=other, stand to school and conservation (01-5)

Table 5-12: School Assignment to Three Levels for Traits on General Practices and Student Activity Opportunities in Conservation at School

This table depicts the assignment of schools to three levels for traits on general practices (gp), student activity opportunities (sa) and on the general held by the principal on school and conservation (o). Several problems with the data from the interview surfaced. One item was not answered by all school and only two categories were necessary for a second item. Perhaps the most unsatisfactory result is that for many of the items the distribution of the twelve schools among the three categories is poorly balanced. Although these categories may represent school reality, comparing the mean scores of categories whose members are so disproportionate may be unreliable measures of these otherwise reasonable categories.

The next section brings together data from the schools (independent variables) and data from the students (dependent variables).

Home Contents

5.4.3. Comparison of student mean scores for extreme groups

Each school has been assigned to one of three levels for each trait representing ecological culture. From each school there is information from students on 1) their activity preferences at school, 2) their knowledge of conservation at school and 3) their environmental motivation. The percentages for each student activity, motivation and knowledge group per school have been calculated (mean score for group "X") and are compared for the extreme groups "high" and "low". For example,

Table 5-13: Sample comparison of the mean scores of dependent (student) variables for the ecological culture extreme groups "high" and "low"

This table shows that for each trait of the major components (features, practices and student participation and standing) the two extreme groups of environmental culture are compared. The "sample mean" indicates the mean value for the student variable for the entire sample of twelve schools. In general, for each trait (independent variable) if scores for the "high" group are higher than for the "low" group, then the student mean scores improve (dependent variable) with the prevalence of this trait. For example, for "Trait1" the mean score for "low" is 0.3 and increases to 0.5. This result suggests an increase in the number of students who "do a bit of everything" at school as the prevalence of "Trait1" increases. "High" schools have on average 50% more students "doing a bit of everything" than "low" schools for this trait. On the other hand, scores indicate that "Trait2" does not favor students who are generally most "active" in conservation at school.

Table 5-14 shows only a portion of the comparison between the extreme groups of ecological culture traits. The complete table is found in the Appendix.

Table 5-14: Mean scores for several dependent (student) variables from the ecological culture extreme groups "high" and "low"

This table, although enormous, gives us the first impressions of effects of school ecological culture on students. Just a glance reveals that many of the traits are associated with variances in student numbers. For example, both design traits (way up at the top of the table) are associated with an increase in the number of students with knowledge scores in conservation at their school of better than 50% and a decrease in the number of students with scores between 26-50%. The number of students with the poorest knowledge scores remains unaffected. I conclude that this is an indication that the design of school grounds - as an integral part of the ecological culture of a school - may influence student knowledge in conservation at their school positively.

Home Contents

5.4.4. Dimensions of ecological culture and students effects

However, instead of continuing trait for trait for all independent variables, I think it makes more sense to get an overall picture on how student groups (dependent variables) are effected by each of the dimensions of ecological culture.

First, all items for a given independent variable, for example, the two "layout" items for the dimension "features and modifications", were compiled and the deviation from the total mean was calculated for the "high" and the "low" groups each (Table 5-15).

Table 5-15: Effects of conservation features and modifications on students for extreme school groups

After the deviations for "high" and "low" groups were calculated, the overall effect of the trait could be calculated in terms of the percent change of students belonging to a given group.

 

Table 5-16: Overall effects of conservation features and modifications on student groups

This table depicts, for example, that as the design of the school grounds improves the number of students who are least active increases (unfortunately) by 24%. The play surfaces account for a reduction of students primarily involved in clean-up activities of 35%. In general, improved features of the school grounds is associated with a 13% decrease in students doing primarily clean-up activities.

(Just two comments about two variables. Funnily enough, the schools who rated poorest for "condition" were visibly the most industrious - "chaotic" some might call it. Nonetheless very busy. They were also located in tougher neighborhoods. Compared to the other schools in the sample they were "messier" but not in comparison with their immediate surroundings. Furthermore, the data on the dependent variable, "sa5" (see Table 7.5 in the APPENDIX), the variable on how students come to school show a consistent contradictory effect to the results that were expected. Students who come to school most ecologically, i.e., with public transportation or on their own steam - as opposed to those driven by their parents, are not more ecological. Just the opposite effect has been observed. This may suggest something about the parent-child relationship and the quality of conservation which is valued which, ironically, is in diametric opposition to the behavior being practiced!)

Then this same procedure was repeated for all variables for the remaining dimensions on ecological culture. The four dimensions of ecological culture found at schools and their overall impact on student variables are summarized in Table 5-17.

 

Table 5-17: Summary of effects of four dimensions of ecological culture on student groups

This table summarizes the general effects on students of the four dimensions used to measure ecological school culture. Ideally, I think one could agree that if ecological culture is a constructive influence on students, then it would be reasonable to expect that the four dimensions of ecological culture ought lead to a decline in the numbers of students in the groups: "little or no", "clean-up", "0-25%", "26-50%", "MOT4" and "MOT3" and an increase for the student groups: "everything", "active", "51-75%", "76-100%", "MOT2" and "MOT1". A description of the actual results follow.

Home Contents

5.4.4.1. Student activity

Effects of the four dimensions on student activity are encouraging but not as straight forward as was hoped. In addition to traits which ought to favor student environmental activity in conservation at school and do, there are other traits with only minimal effects and still others suggest an opposite effect. Expectations were that the four dimensions of ecological culture ought lead to a decline for the groups: "little or no" and "clean-up" activities and an increase for "everything" and "active" student groups. The following table summarizes the observed effects on student activity for each of the four dimensions of ecological culture.

Figure 5-4: Effects of four dimensions of ecological culture on student activity groups

The results can be summarized as follows:

In summary, the results suggest that the dimensions of ecological culture affect numbers of active students and students who do a bit of everything positively and numbers of least active students and those participating in predominantly clean-up activities negatively. Although the results do not, in general, indicate any overt contradiction, the results, however, are not overwhelming.

Home Contents

5.4.4.2. Student conservation knowledge

Effects on student knowledge of conservation at their school are evident and as hoped pleasingly straight forward. Expectations were that the four dimensions of ecological culture ought lead to a decline for the groups "0-25%" and "26-50%" (correct responses) and an increase for the student groups "51-75%" and "76-100%". The following table summarizes the observed effects on student knowledge for each of the four dimensions of ecological culture.

Figure 5-5: Effects of four dimensions of ecological culture on student environmental knowledge groups

The results can be summarized as follows:

All four dimensions of ecological culture:

tend to consistently affect numbers of students who score above 50% positively and numbers of students who score below 50% negatively.

Home Contents

5.4.4.3. Student environmental motivation

The data on effects on student environmental motivation are the most tricky to interpret. It was hoped, as has been suggested many times, that a positive environmental culture would encourage motivation and, therefore, the number of students with the highest motivation scores would increase (MOT1 and MOT2) while the number of students scoring low would decrease (MOT3 and MOT4). Support for this conclusion is not consistent. Rather, the student motivation types responded, in part, unexpectedly to the dimensions of ecological culture as shown in the following figure and described below.

Figure 5-6: Effects of four dimensions of ecological culture on student motivation groups

It was originally thought that as the ecological situation at school improves so would motivation, i.e., there would be a "motivational" shift of students to the right - for all groups. Instead of MOT4 shrinking, this is evidently not the case. MOT4 grows significantly, meaning that a number of students from other groups (which?) have been demotivated. This represents a shift in the wrong direction. In favor of the shift is the growth of MOT1. It cannot be proven with this data, but it almost appears as if MOT1 grows at the expense of MOT2, at least in part. Yet the gap made potentially by students moving from MOT2 into MOT1 is not filled by of MOT3 students experiencing a motivational boost. Sure, the number of MOT3 students does decline but not enough. Furthermore, these students most probably did not land in the MOT2 group as it has shrunk so much.

In summary, the motivation categories do not appear to represent an ordinal continuum - unlike the categories for student conservation knowledge - as has always been suggested. While students of the highest motivational category are, indeed, supported by ecological culture at school, the overall effect for the remaining motivational groups does not run quite according to plan. The second highest motivational group is suppressed; the second lowest is suppressed but only weakly so and the lowest motivation group, in fact, increases.

So far I have established that the dimensions chosen to measure ecological school culture do indeed effect students constructively - for the most part. Now would be a good time to ask what this means for schools as a whole. Does an ecological culture exist? Is ecological culture as a whole useful in explaining student differences at schools? Now I will address these questions and conclude the chapter on results.

Home Contents

5.4.5. Four empirically derived ecological cultures and test of the original hypothesis

Results from the previous section do suggest that the measures of ecological culture may indeed influence student environmental activity, knowledge and motivation. Now I would like to return to the major hypothesis proposed by this investigation, namely that:

Schools which uphold a positive ecological culture:

in turn, support student sensitivity and skills and, therefore, tend to have more students with:

  1. greater knowledge about conservation at their school and
  2. student activity in conservation activities at school resulting in
  3. higher environmental motivation in these students.

For example, it is expected that some schools will score proportionately high for most or all measures of ecological culture. At these "strong" schools, proportionately more students ought to belong to the highest scoring environmental motivation group, have higher than average knowledge of conservation going on at their school and report more experience in environmental activities at school than the remaining students.

At schools whose scores are comparatively low for ecological culture a significantly larger portion of the student body is expected to belong to either of the lowest scoring motivation groups, score significantly lower for their accuracy on conservation at school and report lower activity at school than the others.

Two intermediate ecological cultures remain. But enough on expectations. Let us get on with the actual results. (See the section on Design for a review.)

A cumulative mean score for each of four dimensions on ecological culture was calculated for each school. Each trait is weighted equally. The results of these calculations are recorded in Table 5-18.

Table 5-18: Mean scores for each dimension of ecological culture for 12 schools

The twelve schools were then ranked for each dimension of ecological culture according to the mean score, after which the "better" half was assigned "+" and the "lower" half was assigned "-". Four groups of ecological culture could be identified.

The four types of ecological culture found among the twelve schools in the sample are identified in Table 5-19 below.

Table 5-19: Four ecological cultures found within a sample of twelve schools

It certainly would have been possible to group the six schools in the middle differently. This was tried. In addition, the data was cleaned of items that did not conform to desirable effects on student activity and conservation knowledge and environmental motivation. But none of these attempts changed the end results on student mean scores for the four ecological cultures depicted in Table 5-20 dramatically. So, for this table all items for all dimensions were used. A choice for the middle categories was made primarily to look at the difference between schools that score high for the assessment survey and those schools who score low for the assessment survey (but score high for at least one dimension measured with the assessment interview).


Table 5-20:Mean scores for dependent variables for schools assigned to one of four ecological cultures

Table 5-21 shows these results once again but as deviations from the mean to indicate the overall trend of effect due to the dimensions.

Table 5-21: Effects of four ecological cultures on their student groups

The results indicate that, for example, at schools with a "strong" ecological culture 3% fewer students participated in "little or no" and 5% fewer in "clean-up" activities than the sample means. Conversely, "active" and "a bit of everything" students increased by 5% at these same schools. Although the numbers themselves are not earth shattering, they nevertheless support expectations for schools having a "strong" ecological culture. The following figure depicts the information on all four ecological cultures graphically but for student activity only.

Figure 5-7: Effects of four ecological cultures on their student activity groups

The following figure depicts the information on all four ecological cultures graphically for student knowledge of conservation at their school.

Figure 5-8: Effects of four ecological cultures on their student knowledge groups

The following figure depicts the information on all four ecological cultures graphically for student knowledge of conservation at their school.

Figure 5-9: Effects of four ecological cultures on their student motivation groups

The following summarizes the anticipated and actual results obtained for student effects for each of the four ecological cultures of the sample.

Table 5-22: Summary of anticipated and actual results for the "strong" ecological culture

In summary, the "strong" ecological culture conforms to all expectations but one. Student activity at school, conservation knowledge and student numbers for the highest motivation group are the best of all cultures. The numbers of students, however, belonging to the lowest motivation group (MOT4) are greater and those of the second highest motivation group (MOT2) are lower than the average.

Table 5-23: Summary of anticipated and actual results for the "eco design" ecological culture

In summary, the "eco design" ecological culture fits the description of the expected group originally labeled "standard" the best but instead of high scores for "features" and purely low scores on "practices", their scores for practices were mixed. Scores for these schools were not good enough, tho, to classify them as "strong". Students at these schools do not conform to expectations. Student activity at school is poorest. Contrary to plan, they do not even participate in the activities (clean-up) most commonly associated with school grounds (the dimension at focus here) with above average regularity. Conservation knowledge, as anticipated, is second best, yet only average. This is the only conjunction with expectations. Puzzling is that motivation at these schools is the "best" of all. Proportionately more students are represented in groups MOT1 and MOT2 and fewer in MOT3 and MOT4.

Table 5-24: Summary of anticipated and actual results for the "eco practices" ecological culture

In summary, schools classified as having an "eco practices" culture are a disconcerting. As expected, more of their students feel that they are only cleaning up when it comes to conservation activity at school; and they do indeed have knowledge scores poorer than the mean. In addition to this, motivation is the "poorest" of all ecological cultures. One bright spot is that despite a decline in the number of active students, the number of little or inactive students drops more than for any other ecological culture.

Table 5-25: Summary of anticipated and actual results for the "weak" ecological culture

In summary, this group is miss named. For although they score lowest on the dimensions of ecological culture, this does not typify the students attending these schools. The only expectation students fulfill is that their conservation knowledge is the poorest of all schools. Otherwise their school activity is average - virtually identical with the mean scores. And their motivation is somewhat better than average. They may have fewer students with the highest motivation (MOT1), but they have proportionately more MOT2 students and fewest MOT4 students.

Table 5-26 summarizes the results on student effects that were observed in the four ecological cultures identified at the schools in the follow-up sample.

Table 5-26: Summary of results on the four ecological culture identified in the follow-up sample

This table is a concise summary of the four ecological cultures that were identified in the follow-up study and their distinguishing effects on students. When the actual results found are compared with those that were anticipated, there are a few surprises:

This concludes the section on results. In the next chapter the results will be discussed for their relevance in education and in research.

Home Contents